Indian Journal of Dental ResearchIndian Journal of Dental ResearchIndian Journal of Dental Research
HOME | ABOUT US | EDITORIAL BOARD | AHEAD OF PRINT | CURRENT ISSUE | ARCHIVES | INSTRUCTIONS | SUBSCRIBE | ADVERTISE | CONTACT
Indian Journal of Dental Research   Login   |  Users online:

Home Bookmark this page Print this page Email this page Small font sizeDefault font size Increase font size         

 


 
Table of Contents   
ORIGINAL RESEARCH  
Year : 2017  |  Volume : 28  |  Issue : 2  |  Page : 156-161
Esthetic preferences of laypersons of different cultures and races with regard to smile attractiveness


1 Department of Orthodontics, Faculty of Dentistry, Tabriz University of Medical Sciences, Tabriz, Iran
2 Department of Prosthodontics, Islamic Azad University, Dental Branch of Tehran, Tabriz, Iran
3 Department of Endodontics, Faculty of Dentistry, Tabriz University of Medical Sciences, Tabriz, Iran

Click here for correspondence address and email

Date of Web Publication12-Jun-2017
 

   Abstract 


Background: Culture and ethnicity are among the factors affecting esthetic judgment of individuals. Aims: This study aimed to assess the acceptability threshold of variations in four components of an esthetic smile namely vertical lip thickness, dental midline deviation, buccal corridor, and the golden ratio in maxillary lateral incisors display among laypersons of different races and cultures. Subjects and Methods: Raters (n = 35 in each city) among laypersons of nine cities namely Istanbul, Isfahan, Tabriz, Tehran, Doha, Rome, Sydney, Chicago, and Yazd, were given a photo album containing 27 random images of an attractive female smile, digitally altered with regard to the four smile components. They scored each picture from 0 to 100 in terms of smile attractiveness. Statistical Analysis: Data were analyzed using SPSS 13 and the acceptability threshold for each component was calculated in each city using the Spearman and Wilcoxon tests. P< 0.05 was considered statistically significant. Results: No significant differences were noted with regard to the increased vertical lip thickness, and an acceptability threshold could not be determined for it. The acceptability thresholds for midline deviations, buccal corridor, and the golden ratio were different among different cities. One-millimeter increase in the displayed width of maxillary lateral incisors was more desirable than the golden ratio standard width. Conclusion: Culture and race may significantly affect the esthetic preference of individuals with regard to smile attractiveness.

Keywords: Culture, esthetic preference, race, smile attractiveness

How to cite this article:
Sadrhaghighi AH, Zarghami A, Sadrhaghighi S, Mohammadi A, Eskandarinezhad M. Esthetic preferences of laypersons of different cultures and races with regard to smile attractiveness. Indian J Dent Res 2017;28:156-61

How to cite this URL:
Sadrhaghighi AH, Zarghami A, Sadrhaghighi S, Mohammadi A, Eskandarinezhad M. Esthetic preferences of laypersons of different cultures and races with regard to smile attractiveness. Indian J Dent Res [serial online] 2017 [cited 2023 Mar 21];28:156-61. Available from: https://www.ijdr.in/text.asp?2017/28/2/156/207795



   Introduction Top


Several components are known to be involved in an esthetic smile.[1],[2] However, beauty is a subjective concept. Evidence shows that the standards of beauty are widely variable depending on the social and cultural background, age, gender, ethnicity, and race of observers.[3],[4] Norms of beauty are constantly changing, and smile esthetics would not be an exception.[5]

Smile attractiveness factors have been the subject of many studies.[1], 2, [6],[7],[8],[9] Maxillary midline deviation significantly compromises smile attractiveness.[10],[11] Another critical feature is the optimal size of buccal corridors.[2], 6, [9],[10],[11] Alterations in vertical lip thickness have also been shown to affect smile esthetics.[12] The role of the golden ratio standards in smile attractiveness has been discussed in several studies as well.[6],[13] Despite attempts, a consensus on acceptability thresholds for variations in esthetic smile components has not yet been reached.

Perspectives of laypersons with regard to smile esthetics have been compared in many previous studies.[6], 8, [14],[15],[16] However, to the best of our knowledge, the number of studies comparing the esthetic preferences of individuals of different races and cultural background with regard to smile attractiveness is scarce.[3],[5],[17] Thus, this study aimed to assess the perspectives of raters from nine cities worldwide with regard to changes in special factors influencing smile attractiveness and determine an acceptability threshold for each.


   Subjects and Methods Top


This descriptive, analytical study was conducted on laypersons selected among male and female ethnic residents of nine cities in the age range of 18–45 years with native, ethnic parents living in the same city. A total of 35 laypersons were selected from each city according to a similar previous study.[18]

A picture of an attractive female smile measuring 2.5” × 5” with 1000 pixels resolution was selected by unanimous agreement of three orthodontists.[19] Using Photoshop CS3 (Adobe Systems Inc., San Jose, CA), the following smile components were digitally manipulated:

  1. Maxillary dental midline was deviated by 1 mm toward the right and left relative to the upper lip vermilion
  2. Size of buccal corridor was altered by changing the arch width posterior to the canine to narrow or widen the arch width by 10%
  3. Increasing the vertical lip thickness at the vermilion by 1 mm
  4. Changing the golden ratio in the maxillary lateral incisors by increasing or decreasing the teeth width symmetrically by 1 mm.


The pictures were converted to black and white.[20] A total of 27 digitally manipulated images were randomly arranged in a photo album grouped into four series [21] [Figure 1]. After each series of photos (related to each altered smile component), a landscape image was placed to prevent tiredness of observers [18] [Table 1], [Table 2], [Table 3], [Table 4].
Figure 1: Randomly arranged images

Click here to view
Table  1: Random arrangement of images showing midline deviation

Click here to view
Table  2: Random arrangement of images showing buccal corridor alterations

Click here to view
Table  3: Random arrangement of images showing vertical lip thickness manipulation

Click here to view
Table  4: Random arrangement of images showing golden ratio manipulation

Click here to view


A questionnaire was prepared in Farsi and English, containing demographic information of raters including age, sex, place of birth of the rater and parents, and the right-handedness or left-handedness. The next pages contained 27 horizontal 10 cm long visual analog scales.[22] The photo album, the questionnaire, and related instructions were mailed to all participants in Sydney (Australia), Rome (Italy), Chicago (US), Doha (Qatar), Istanbul (Turkey) and Tabriz, Tehran, Isfahan, and Yazd (Iran). Cases gave their informed consent for participation. They were instructed to first take a look at all images from 30 cm distance under adequate daylight without pausing on a specific image or comparing them.[23] In the second review, they rated the images one by one from 0 to 100 based on attractiveness without any comparison between them.[21] Subjects were free to quit.

Data were analyzed using SPSS 13 (Microsoft, IL, USA). The Cronbach's alpha was calculated to assess the reliability of results in each city.[8] For this purpose, the kappa statistic was calculated for the four duplicate images (#3, 11, 16, 22) randomly placed among other images.[13],[18],[24] The reliability of the results was excellent (>0.6) except for Doha with moderate reliability [Table 5].
Table  5: Reliability assessment: The Cronbach's alpha coefficient

Click here to view


The Spearman's correlation test was applied to analyze the correlation of scores for each photograph with the score of the original image. The acceptability thresholds were calculated for raters in each city using the Wilcoxon test. P < 0.05 was considered statistically significant.


   Results Top


A total of 293 subjects (135 males, 158 females) participated in this study. Participants were in the age range of 17–46 years. 221 were right-handed, and 72 were left-handed.

Midline deviation

In Istanbul, significant correlations were found between S3 (reference picture) and S4 and S7 (P< 0.05). Therefore, the acceptability threshold for midline deviation was found to be 2 mm. The difference in the mean scores given to images S1 to S9 was not significant when compared to S3 (P > 0.05).

In Isfahan, a significant correlation was found between S3 and S7. Therefore, the acceptability threshold for midline deviation was found to be 1 mm. The difference in the mean scores given to images S1, S2, S5–S7, and S9 was significant when compared to S3 (P< 0.05).

In Tabriz, significant correlations were found between S3 and S1, S2, S4–S8. Therefore, the acceptability threshold for midline deviation was found to be 2 mm. The difference in the mean scores given to images S1, S2, S5, S6, and S9 was significant when compared to S3 (P< 0.05).

In Tehran, no association was found between S3 and S1, S2, S4–S9 (P > 0.05). Therefore, the acceptability threshold could not be determined.

In Doha, significant correlations were found between S3 and S2, S4, S5, and S8 (P< 0.05). Therefore, the acceptability threshold for midline deviation was found to be 3 mm. The difference in the mean scores given to images S1–S9 was not significant when compared to S3 (P > 0.05).

In Rome, significant correlations were found between S3 and S4, S7, and S8. Therefore, the acceptability threshold for midline deviation was found to be 2 mm. The difference in the mean scores given to S1, S2, S4–S7, and S9 was significant when compared to S3 (P< 0.05).

In Sydney, significant correlations existed between S3 and S1, S2, S4, S5–S9 (P< 0.05). Therefore, the acceptability threshold could not be determined.

In Chicago, significant correlations existed between S3 and S1, S2, S4, S6–S9 (P< 0.05). Therefore, the acceptability threshold could not be determined.

In Yazd, significant correlations were found between S3 and S4 and S8. Therefore, the acceptability threshold for midline deviation was found to be 2 mm. The difference in the mean scores to S5 and S6 was significant when compared to S3 (P< 0.05).

Buccal corridor

In Istanbul, significant correlations were found between S11 (reference picture) and S10, S12–S15 (P< 0.05). Therefore, the acceptability threshold could not be determined.

In Isfahan, S11 had a significant correlation with S10, S14, and S15. Therefore, the acceptability threshold for variations in buccal corridor was found to be 20%. The difference in the mean scores given to S12–S15 was significant when compared to S11 (P< 0.05).

In Tabriz, significant associations were found between S11 and S10, S12–S15. Therefore, the acceptability threshold for variations in buccal corridor was found to be 10%. The difference in the mean scores given to S12–S15 was significant when compared to S11 (P< 0.05).

In Tehran, a significant association was found between S11 and S15 (P< 0.05). Thus, the acceptability threshold could not be determined.

In Doha, a significant association was found between S11 and S10. Thus, the acceptability threshold for variations in buccal corridor was found to be 10%. The difference in the mean scores given to S10, S13–S15 was significant when compared to S11 (P< 0.05).

In Rome, significant associations were found between S11 and S10, S13–S15. Therefore, the acceptability threshold for variations in buccal corridor was found to be 40%. The difference in the mean scores given to S12–S15 was significant when compared to S11 (P< 0.05).

In Sydney, significant associations were found between S11 and S10 and S12–S15. Therefore, the acceptability threshold for variations in buccal corridor was found to be 10%. The difference in the mean scores given to S12 and S13–S15 was significant when compared to S11 (P< 0.05).

In Chicago, significant associations were found between S11 and S10 and S14. Therefore, the acceptability threshold for variations in buccal corridor was found to be 20%. The difference in the mean scores given to S12–S15 was significant when compared to S11 (P< 0.05).

In Yazd, significant associations were found between S11 and S10, S12–S15. Therefore, the acceptability threshold for variations in buccal corridor was found to be 10%. The difference in the mean scores given to S12–S15 was significant when compared to S11 (P< 0.05).

Vertical lip thickness

In Istanbul, significant associations were found between S16 (reference picture) and S17–S19 (P< 0.05). Thus, the acceptability threshold could not be calculated.

In Isfahan, significant associations were found between S16 and S17–S20 (P< 0.05). Thus, the acceptability threshold could not be calculated.

In Tabriz, significant associations were found between S16 and S17–S20 (P< 0.05). Thus, the acceptability threshold could not be calculated.

In Tehran, significant associations were found between S16 and S17 and S20 (P< 0.05). Thus, the acceptability threshold for variations in vertical lip thickness was found to be 4 mm. The difference in the mean scores given to S16–S20 was not significant (P > 0.05).

In Doha, significant associations were found between S16 and S17, S18, and S20 (P< 0.05). Thus, the acceptability threshold for variations in vertical lip thickness was found to be 3 mm. The difference in the mean scores given to S16–S20 was not significant (P > 0.05).

In Rome, significant associations were found between S16 and S17–S20 (P< 0.05). Thus, the acceptability threshold could not be determined.

In Sydney, significant associations were found between S16 and S17 and S18. Thus, the acceptability threshold for variations in vertical lip thickness was found to be 3 mm. The difference in the mean scores given to S18, S19, and S20 was significant when compared to S16 (P< 0.05).

In Chicago, significant associations were found between S16 and S17–S20 (P< 0.05). Thus, the acceptability threshold could not be determined.

In Yazd, significant associations were found between S16 and S17–S19 (P< 0.05). Thus, the acceptability threshold could not be determined.

Golden ratio

In Istanbul, significant associations were found between S22 (reference picture) and S21, S23, and S26 (P< 0.05). Thus, the acceptability threshold for variations in the golden ratio of the maxillary lateral incisors was reduction by 3 mm. The difference in the mean scores given to S25–S27 (P = 0.00) was significant when compared to S22.

In Isfahan, significant associations were found between S22 and S21, S23–S26. Thus, the acceptability threshold for variations in the golden ratio of the maxillary lateral incisors was its increase or reduction by 2 mm. The difference in the mean scores given to S21, S23–S27 was significant when compared to S22 (P< 0.05).

In Tabriz, significant associations were found between S22 and S21, S23–S27 (P< 0.05). Thus, the acceptability threshold could not be determined.

In Tehran, significant associations were found between S22 and S21, S23–S25. Thus, the acceptability threshold for variations in the golden ratio of the maxillary lateral incisors was increased by 2 mm and decreased by 1 mm. The difference in the mean scores given to S21, S23, S26, and S27 was significant when compared to S22 (P< 0.05).

In Doha, significant associations were found between S22 and S23–S26. Thus, the acceptability threshold for variations in the golden ratio of the maxillary lateral incisors was increased by 2 mm and decreased by 2 mm. The difference in the mean scores given to S21 and S23 was significant when compared to S22 (P< 0.05).

In Rome, significant associations were found between S22 and S21, S23, and S26 (P< 0.05). Thus, the acceptability threshold for variations in the golden ratio of the maxillary lateral incisors was reduction by 3 mm. The difference in the mean scores given to S21 (P = 0.00) was significant when compared to S22.

In Sydney, significant associations were found between S22 and S21, S23–S27 (P< 0.05). Thus, the acceptability threshold could not be determined.

In Chicago, significant associations were found between S22 and S21, S23, S24, and S26 (P< 0.05). Thus, the acceptability threshold could not be determined.

In Yazd, significant associations were found between S22 and S21, S23, and S26. Thus, the acceptability threshold for variations in the golden ratio for maxillary lateral incisors was reduction by 3 mm. The difference in the mean scores given to S21, S23, and S26 was significant when compared to S22 (P< 0.05).

Summary of the acceptability thresholds in different cities is shown in [Table 6].
Table 6: The acceptability thresholds in different cities

Click here to view



   Discussion Top


Assessments of smile components have been done in the previous studies by rating photographs of smiles showing only teeth and lips.[20] To enable comparison among different cities, the effect of demographic factors was adjusted for as potential confounders.[23],[25] In this study in agreement with some other studies, randomly placed images in four series prevented the raters to notice the incremental trend of altered components and probably made a more accurate judgment.[5],[18],[20],[21] Moreover, black and white pictures would eliminate the confounding effect of skin color on judgment.[20]

Inter-observer reliability (>0.6) was among the strengths of the current study. Only in Doha, results had moderate reliability, which may be due to less attention paid to instructions by the judges. In a previous study with similar methodology, moderate reliability (0.3–0.6) was reported except for the buccal corridor.[20] In another study by Chang et al. reliability of all results except for buccal corridor was reported to be moderate to excellent (>0.3).[18]

In the current study, judges were asked to rate an esthetic smile with 4 modified components.

Small midline deviations have been reported to be undetectable by laypersons.[16],[21],[26] Based on our results, the acceptability threshold of midline deviation was 2–3 mm in Istanbul, Tabriz, Isfahan, Doha, Rome, and Yazd. The acceptability threshold in Tehran, Sydney, and Chicago could not be determined. Future studies are required on a larger sample size to better elucidate this subject. Moreover, the method of study must be more thoroughly explained to layperson judges. The difference in the threshold of acceptability in Doha (3 mm) and other cities (2 mm) may show different esthetic preferences of raters residing in different geographical locations. Janson et al. reported that dental midline deviation by 2.2 mm was esthetically acceptable by orthodontists and laypersons.[26] McLeod et al. reported that the acceptability threshold of midline deviation for Canadians was 1.1 mm less than that for Americans.[25] Pinho et al. in Brazil showed that laypersons did not notice midline deviations.[21] Zhang et al. reported an acceptable threshold of 1–3 mm for midline deviation in China [27] and Parekh et al. reported an acceptability threshold of 3.2 mm in the United States.[8] These values along with our findings may show the effect of race and ethnicity on esthetic preferences. However, as these studies did not aim to assess the effect of ethnicity on esthetic perception the inclusion criteria of raters might be a confounding factor.

Effect of right-handedness or left-handedness of the raters on their judgment with regard to the side of midline deviation was suggested by Thomas et al.[28] Zhang et al. found no significant association between the side of deviation and scores allocated to images.[27] We evaluated midline deviation to both right and left sides. However, due to small number of left-handed individuals, we could not statistically assess this effect and future studies are required to better elucidate this subject.

Janson et al. showed that excessive buccal corridor was mainly the reason behind an unesthetic smile.[26] In our study, an acceptability threshold for variations in buccal corridor could not be determined in Istanbul and Tehran. However, this was 20% in Isfahan and Chicago, 40% in Rome, and 10% in Tabriz, Doha, and Yazd. These findings indicate the variability in esthetic preferences of individuals in different cities. Ioi et al. in Japan reported an acceptability threshold of 10% for the size of buccal corridor.[22] McLeod et al. showed that the acceptability threshold was 19.5% in Canada and 22% in the United States.[25] Moore et al., in the US reported this value to be 10%.[6] Overall, race and ethnicity seem to have significant effects on raters' esthetic judgment with regard to alterations in buccal corridor.

Raters in Sydney and Doha preferred fuller lips. The difference in this regard among other raters was not significant. McNamara et al., in the UK showed that raters believed that fuller lips were more attractive.[12] Considering the scarcity of studies on this topic, further investigations seem necessary.

In this study, the golden ratio in the maxillary lateral incisors was modified by both decreasing and increasing the teeth widths. An interesting finding was that 1 mm increase in the width of the maxillary lateral incisors improved smile attractiveness according to the opinions of the judges in all cities. In some areas, 2–3 mm increase was even more desirable. This finding supports the results of Kokich et al. in the US that the golden ratio may not be accurate enough with respect to the width of maxillary lateral incisors in symmetric assessments.[6],[13] In Chicago, Sydney, and Tabriz, an acceptability threshold could not be determined. Results in other cities were widely variable and showed the effect of race and ethnicity on esthetic judgment with regard to changes in the golden ratio and its role in smile attractiveness. Considering the lack of similar studies, future studies are required.


   Conclusion Top


Ethnicity, race, and culture may significantly affect the esthetic judgment of individuals. The golden ratio must not be necessarily adhered to when it comes to the width of the maxillary lateral incisors to maximize esthetics.

Financial support and sponsorship

Nil.

Conflicts of interest

There are no conflicts of interest.



 
   References Top

1.
Sarver DM. Principles of cosmetic dentistry in orthodontics: Part 1. Shape and proportionality of anterior teeth. Am J Orthod Dentofacial Orthop 2004;126:749-53.  Back to cited text no. 1
[PUBMED]    
2.
Proffit W, Fields HW, Sarver DM. Contemporary Orthodontics. 5th ed. St. Louis, Mo: Elsevier/Mosby; 2013.  Back to cited text no. 2
    
3.
Oumeish OY. The cultural and philosophical concepts of cosmetics in beauty and art through the medical history of mankind. Clin Dermatol 2001;19:375-86.  Back to cited text no. 3
[PUBMED]    
4.
Liu Y, Korn EL, Oh HS, Pearson H, Xu TM, Baumrind S. Comparison of Chinese and US orthodontists' averaged evaluations of “facial attractiveness” from end-of-treatment facial photographs. Am J Orthod Dentofacial Orthop 2009;135:621-34.  Back to cited text no. 4
    
5.
Nomura M, Motegi E, Hatch JP, Gakunga PT, Ng'ang'a PM, Rugh JD, et al. Esthetic preferences of European American, Hispanic American, Japanese, and African judges for soft-tissue profiles. Am J Orthod Dentofacial Orthop 2009;135 4 Suppl: S87-95.  Back to cited text no. 5
    
6.
Moore T, Southard KA, Casko JS, Qian F, Southard TE. Buccal corridors and smile esthetics. Am J Orthod Dentofacial Orthop 2005;127:208-13.  Back to cited text no. 6
    
7.
Kokich VO Jr., Kiyak HA, Shapiro PA. Comparing the perception of dentists and lay people to altered dental esthetics. J Esthet Dent 1999;11:311-24.  Back to cited text no. 7
    
8.
Parekh SM, Fields HW, Beck M, Rosenstiel S. Attractiveness of variations in the smile arc and buccal corridor space as judged by orthodontists and laymen. Angle Orthod 2006;76:557-63.  Back to cited text no. 8
    
9.
Wong NK, Kassim AA, Foong KW. Analysis of esthetic smiles by using computer vision techniques. Am J Orthod Dentofacial Orthop 2005;128:404-11.  Back to cited text no. 9
    
10.
Nanda R, Margolis MJ. Treatment strategies for midline discrepancies. Semin Orthod 1996;2:84-9.  Back to cited text no. 10
    
11.
Arnett GW, Bergman RT. Facial keys to orthodontic diagnosis and treatment planning – Part II. Am J Orthod Dentofacial Orthop 1993;103:395-411.  Back to cited text no. 11
    
12.
McNamara L, McNamara JA Jr., Ackerman MB, Baccetti T. Hard-and soft-tissue contributions to the esthetics of the posed smile in growing patients seeking orthodontic treatment. Am J Orthod Dentofacial Orthop 2008;133:491-9.  Back to cited text no. 12
    
13.
Kokich VO, Kokich VG, Kiyak HA. Perceptions of dental professionals and laypersons to altered dental esthetics: Asymmetric and symmetric situations. Am J Orthod Dentofacial Orthop 2006;130:141-51.  Back to cited text no. 13
    
14.
Gracco A, Cozzani M, D'Elia L, Manfrini M, Peverada C, Siciliani G. The smile buccal corridors: Aesthetic value for dentists and laypersons. Prog Orthod 2006;7:56-65.  Back to cited text no. 14
    
15.
Gul-e-Erum, Fida M. Changes in smile parameters as perceived by orthodontists, dentists, artists, and laypeople. World J Orthod 2008;9:132-40.  Back to cited text no. 15
    
16.
Talic N, Alomar S, Almaidhan A. Perception of Saudi dentists and lay people to altered smile esthetics. Saudi Dent J 2013;25:13-21.  Back to cited text no. 16
    
17.
Ioi H, Kang S, Shimomura T, Kim SS, Park SB, Son WS, et al. Effects of vertical positions of anterior teeth on smile esthetics in Japanese and korean orthodontists and orthodontic patients. J Esthet Restor Dent 2013;25:274-82.  Back to cited text no. 17
    
18.
Chang CA, Fields HW Jr., Beck FM, Springer NC, Firestone AR, Rosenstiel S, et al. Smile esthetics from patients' perspectives for faces of varying attractiveness. Am J Orthod Dentofacial Orthop 2011;140:e171-80.  Back to cited text no. 18
    
19.
Roden-Johnson D, Gallerano R, English J. The effects of buccal corridor spaces and arch form on smile esthetics. Am J Orthod Dentofacial Orthop 2005;127:343-50.  Back to cited text no. 19
    
20.
Springer NC, Chang C, Fields HW, Beck FM, Firestone AR, Rosenstiel S, et al. Smile esthetics from the layperson's perspective. Am J Orthod Dentofacial Orthop 2011;139:e91-101.  Back to cited text no. 20
    
21.
Pinho S, Ciriaco C, Faber J, Lenza MA. Impact of dental asymmetries on the perception of smile esthetics. Am J Orthod Dentofacial Orthop 2007;132:748-53.  Back to cited text no. 21
    
22.
Ioi H, Nakata S, Counts AL. Effects of buccal corridors on smile esthetics in Japanese. Angle Orthod 2009;79:628-33.  Back to cited text no. 22
    
23.
Kim E, Gianelly AA. Extraction vs nonextraction: Arch widths and smile esthetics. Angle Orthod 2003;73:354-8.  Back to cited text no. 23
    
24.
Lombardi RE. The principles of visual perception and their clinical application to denture esthetics. J Prosthet Dent 1973;29:358-82.  Back to cited text no. 24
    
25.
McLeod C, Fields HW, Hechter F, Wiltshire W, Rody W Jr., Christensen J. Esthetics and smile characteristics evaluated by laypersons. Angle Orthod 2011;81:198-205.  Back to cited text no. 25
    
26.
Janson G, Branco NC, Fernandes TM, Sathler R, Garib D, Lauris JR. Influence of orthodontic treatment, midline position, buccal corridor and smile arc on smile attractiveness. Angle Orthod 2011;81:153-61.  Back to cited text no. 26
    
27.
Zhang YF, Xiao L, Li J, Peng YR, Zhao Z. Young people's esthetic perception of dental midline deviation. Angle Orthod 2010;80:515-20.  Back to cited text no. 27
    
28.
Thomas JL, Hayes C, Zawaideh S. The effect of axial midline angulation on dental esthetics. Angle Orthod 2003;73:359-64.  Back to cited text no. 28
    

Top
Correspondence Address:
Afsaneh Zarghami
Department of Orthodontics, Faculty of Dentistry, Tabriz University of Medical Sciences, Tabriz
Iran
Login to access the Email id

Source of Support: None, Conflict of Interest: None


DOI: 10.4103/0970-9290.207795

Rights and Permissions


    Figures

  [Figure 1]
 
 
    Tables

  [Table 1], [Table 2], [Table 3], [Table 4], [Table 5], [Table 6]

This article has been cited by
1 Comparison between orthodontist, general dentist, and layperson in the perception of pink esthetics on smile: A cross-sectional study
Nivedita Sharma, Sridhar Kannan, Nitin Arora, Ashish Kumar Singh, Abhita Malhotra, Panchali Batra
APOS Trends in Orthodontics. 2022; 0: 1
[Pubmed] | [DOI]
2 Aesthetic crown lengthening and lip repositioning surgery: Pre- and post-operative assessment of smile attractiveness
Cléverson O. Silva, Robson I. Rezende, Amanda C. Mazuquini, Vinicius C. Leal, Gislaine S. A. Amaral, Xiaohan Guo, Dimitris N. Tatakis
Journal of Clinical Periodontology. 2021; 48(6): 826
[Pubmed] | [DOI]
3 A study of smile aesthetic perception among dental professionals, patients and parents towards impacted maxillary canine treatment options
Leonie Seager, Jinesh Shah, Trevor Burke, Balvinder Khambay
Journal of Orthodontics. 2021; 48(3): 250
[Pubmed] | [DOI]
4

Perception of Altered Smile Esthetics by Orthodontists, Dentists, and Laypeople in Riyadh, Saudi Arabia

Arwa Ahmed Aldeeri, Khaled Abdullah Alhababi, Fahad Abdullah Algahtani, Abrar Abdullah Tounsi, Khalid Ibrahim Albadr
Clinical, Cosmetic and Investigational Dentistry. 2020; Volume 12: 563
[Pubmed] | [DOI]



 

Top
 
 
 
  Search
 
    Similar in PUBMED
   Search Pubmed for
   Search in Google Scholar for
 Related articles
    Email Alert *
    Add to My List *
* Registration required (free)  
 


    Abstract
   Introduction
   Subjects and Methods
   Results
   Discussion
   Conclusion
    References
    Article Figures
    Article Tables

 Article Access Statistics
    Viewed7088    
    Printed496    
    Emailed0    
    PDF Downloaded60    
    Comments [Add]    
    Cited by others 4    

Recommend this journal